What does a Bayes factor feel like?(转)
A Bayes factor (BF) is a statistical index that quantifies the evidence for a hypothesis, compared to an alternative hypothesis (for introductions to Bayes factors, see here, here or here).
Although the BF is a continuous measure of evidence, humans love verbal labels, categories, and benchmarks. Labels give interpretations of the objective index – and that is both the good and the bad about labels. The good thing is that these labels can facilitate communication (but see @richardmorey), and people just crave for verbal interpretations to guide their understanding of those “boring” raw numbers.

The bad thing about labels is that an interpretation should always be context dependent (Such as “30 min.” can be both a long time (train delay) or a short time (concert), as @CaAl said). But once a categorical system has been established, it’s no longer context dependent.
These labels can also be a dangerous tool, as they implicitly introduce cutoff values (“Hey, the BF jumped over the boundary of 3. It’s not anecdotal any more, it’s moderate evidence!”). But wedo not want another sacred .05 criterion!; see also Andrew Gelman’s blog post and its critical comments. The strength of the BF is precisely its non-binary nature.
Several labels for paraphrasing the size of a BF have been suggested. The most common system seems to be the suggestion of Harold Jeffreys (1961):
Bayes factor |
Label |
---|---|
> 100 | Extreme evidence for H1 |
30 – 100 | Very strong evidence for H1 |
10 – 30 | Strong evidence for H1 |
3 – 10 | Moderate evidence for H1 |
1 – 3 | Anecdotal evidence for H1 |
1 | No evidence |
1/3 – 1 | Anecdotal evidence for H0 |
1/3 – 1/10 | Moderate evidence for H0 |
1/10 – 1/30 | Strong evidence for H0 |
1/30 – 1/100 | Very strong evidence for H0 |
< 1/100 | Extreme evidence for H0 |
Note: The original label for 3 < BF < 10 was “substantial evidence”. Lee and Wagenmakers (2013) changed it to “moderate”, as “substantial” already sounds too decisive. “Anecdotal” formerly was known as “Barely worth mentioning”.
Kass and Raftery suggested a comparable classification, only that the “strong evidence” category for them starts at BF > 20 (see also Wikipedia entry).
Getting a feeling for Bayes factors
How much is a of 3.7? It indicates that data occured 3.7x more likely under
than under
, given the priors assumed in the model. Is that a lot of evidence for
? Or not?
Following Table 1, it can be labeled “moderate evidence” for an effect – whatever that means.
Some have argued that strong evidence, such as BFs > 10, are quite evident from eyeballing only:
“If your result needs a statistician then you should design a better experiment.” (attributed to Ernest Rutherford)
If you have to search for the statistically significant, then it’s not. #statistics #ddj#dataviz
— Edward Tufte (@EdwardTufte) 13. Januar 2015
Is that really the case? Can we just “see” it when there is an effect?
Visualizing Bayes factors for proportions
Imagine the following scenario: When I give a present to my two boys (4 and 6 years old), it is not so important what it is. The most important thing is: “Is it fair?”. (And my boys are very sensitive detectors of unfairness).
Imagine you have bags with red and blue marbles. Obviously, the blue marbles are much better, so it is key to make sure that in each bag there is an equal number of red and blue marbles. Hence, for our familial harmony I should check whether reds and blues are distributed evenly or not. In statistical terms: : p = 0.5,
: p != 0.5.
When drawing samples from the bags, the strongest evidence for an even distribution () is given when exactly the same number of red and blue marbles has been drawn. How much evidence for
is it when I draw n=2, 1 red/1 blue? The answer is in Figure 1, upper table, first row: The
is 0.86 in favor of
, resp. a
of 1.16 in favor of
– i.e., anecdotal evidence for an equal distribution.
You can get these values easily with the famous BayesFactor package for R:
What if I had drawn two reds instead? Then the BF would be 1.14 in favor of (see Figure 1, lower table, row 1).
Obviously, with small sample sizes it’s not possible to generate strong evidence, neither for nor for
. You need a minimal sample size to leave the region of “anecdotal evidence”. Figure 1 shows some examples how the BF gets more extreme with increasing sample size.

Figure 1.
These visualizations indeed seem to indicate that for simple designs such as the urn model you do not really need a statistical test if your BF is > 10. You can just see it from looking at the data (although the “obviousness” is more pronounced for large BFs in small sample sizes).
Maximal and minimal Bayes factors for a certain sample size
The dotted lines in Figure 2 show the maximal and the minimal BF that can be obtained for a given number of drawn marbles. The minimum BF is obtained when the sample is maximally consistent with (i.e. when exactly the same number of red and blue marbles has been drawn), the maximal BF is obtained when only marbles from one color are drawn.

Figure 2: Maximal and minimal BF for a certain sample size.
Figure 2 highlights two features:
- If you have few data points, you cannot have strong evidence, neither for
nor for
.
- It is much easier to get strong evidence for
than for
. This property depends somewhat on the choice of the prior distribution of
effect sizes. If you expect very strong effects under the
, it is easier to get evidence for
. But still, with every reasonable prior distribution, it is easier to gather evidence for
.
Get a feeling yourself!
Here’s a shiny widget that let’s you draw marbles from the urn. Monitor how the BF evolves as you sequentially add marbles to your sample!

Teaching sequential sampling and Bayes factors
When I teach sequential sampling and Bayes factors, I bring an actual bag with marbles (or candies of two colors).
In my typical setup I ask some volunteers to test whether the same amount of both colors is in the bag. (The bag of course has a cover so that they don’t see the marbles). They may sample as many marbles as they want, but each marble costs them 10 Cent (i.e., an efficiency criterium: Sample as much as necessary, but not too much!). They should think aloud, about when they have a first hunch, and when they are relatively sure about the presence or absence of an effect. I use a color mixture of 2:1 – in my experience this give a good chance to detect the difference, but it’s not too obvious (some teams stop sampling and conclude “no difference”).
This exercise typically reveals following insights (hopefully!)
- By intuition, humans sample sequentially. When the evidence is not strong enough, more data is sampled, until they are sure enough about the (un)fairness of the distribution.
- Intuitionally, nobody does a fixed-n design with a-priori power analysis.
- Often, they stop quite soon, in the range of “anecdotal evidence”. It’s also my own impression: BFs that are still in the “anecdotal” range already look quite conclusive for everyday hypothesis testing (e.g., a 2 vs. 9 distribution;
= 2.7). This might change, however, if in the scenario a wrong decision is associated with higher costs. Next time, I will try a scenario of prescription drugs which have potentially severe side effects.
The “interocular traumatic test”
The analysis so far seems to support the “interocular traumatic test”: “when the data are so compelling that conclusion hits you straight between the eyes” (attributed to Joseph Berkson; quoted from Wagenmakers, Verhagen, & Ly, 2014).
But the authors go on and quote Edwards et al. (1963, p. 217), who said: “…the enthusiast’s interocular trauma may be the skeptic’s random error. A little arithmetic to verify the extent of the trauma can yield great peace of mind for little cost.”.
In the next visualization we will see, that large Bayes factors are not always obvious.
Visualizing Bayes factors for group differences
What happens if we switch to group differences? European women have on average a self-reported height of 165.8 cm, European males of 177.9 cm – difference: 12.1 cm, pooled standard deviation is around 7 cm. (Source:European Community Household Panel; see Garcia, J., & Quintana-Domeque, C., 2007; based on ~50,000 participants born between 1970 and 1980). This translates to a Cohen’s d of 1.72.
Unfortunately, this source only contains self-reported heights, which can be subject to biases (males over-report their height on average). But it was the only source I found which also contains the standard deviations within sex. However, Meyer et al (2001)report a similar effect size of d = 1.8 for objectively measured heights.
Now look at this plot. Would you say the blue lines are obviously higher than the red ones?
I couldn’t say for sure. But the is 14.54, a “strong” evidence!
If we sort the lines by height the effect is more visible:
… and alternatively, we can plot the distributions of males’ and females’ heights:
Again, you can play around with the interactive app:

Can we get a feeling for Bayes factors?
To summarize: Whether a strong evidence “hits you between the eyes” depends on many things – the kind of test, the kind of visualization, the sample size. Sometimes a BF of 2.5 seems obvious, and sometimes it is hard to spot a BF>100 by eyeballing only. Overall, I’m glad that we have a numeric measure of strength of evidence and do not have to rely on eyeballing only.
Try it yourself – draw some marbles in the interactive app, or change the height difference between males and females, and calibrate your personal gut feeling with the resulting Bayes factor!
转自:http://www.nicebread.de/what-does-a-bayes-factor-feel-like/
What does a Bayes factor feel like?(转)的更多相关文章
- [Bayes] Understanding Bayes: Visualization of the Bayes Factor
From: https://alexanderetz.com/2015/08/09/understanding-bayes-visualization-of-bf/ Nearly被贝叶斯因子搞死,找篇 ...
- Bayes factor
bayes因子为什么一定要除以先验机会比,如果是想用样本的作用,来判断支持原来的假设θ_0,H_0的力度,直接用后验概率比不就好了吗? 左边等于右边
- [Bayes] Understanding Bayes: A Look at the Likelihood
From: https://alexanderetz.com/2015/04/15/understanding-bayes-a-look-at-the-likelihood/ Reading note ...
- vcf_filter.py
pyvcf 中带的一个工具 比其他工具用着好些 其他filter我很信不过~~ 自己写的功能又很有限 所以转投vcf_filter.py啦 Filtering a VCF file based on ...
- 本人AI知识体系导航 - AI menu
Relevant Readable Links Name Interesting topic Comment Edwin Chen 非参贝叶斯 徐亦达老板 Dirichlet Process 学习 ...
- Machine Learning——吴恩达机器学习笔记(酷
[1] ML Introduction a. supervised learning & unsupervised learning 监督学习:从给定的训练数据集中学习出一个函数(模型参数), ...
- Random/Stochastic
---恢复内容开始--- ===================================================== A random variable's possible valu ...
- PRML-Chapter3 Linear Models for Regression
Example: Polynomial Curve Fitting The goal of regression is to predict the value of one or more cont ...
- ggstatsplot绘图|统计+可视化,学术科研神器
本文首发于“生信补给站”公众号,https://mp.weixin.qq.com/s/zdSit97SOEpbnR18ARzixw 更多关于R语言,ggplot2绘图,生信分析的内容,敬请关注小号. ...
随机推荐
- .Net 官方学习文档
.Net 官方学习文档:https://docs.microsoft.com/zh-cn/dotnet/articles/welcome
- javascript核心概念之——数组
在javascript中数组就是一个可以存放任何类型的集合.存储在数组中的值用逗号分隔 var arr = ["hello",7,null,undifined,obj,undifi ...
- 通过一个小游戏开始接触Python!
之前就一直嚷嚷着要找视频看学习Python,可是一直拖到今晚才开始....好好加油吧骚年,坚持不一定就能有好的结果,但是不坚持就一定是不好的!! 看着视频学习1: 首先,打开IDLE,在IDLE中新建 ...
- js继承之原型链方式实现
温故而知新: 在之前的文章已经重点了解了原型对象,今天就理一理这个原型对象在原型链式的继承中的使用 function a(x,y){this.x=x;this.y=y;} //定义一个函数,当构造函数 ...
- 使用HttpClient进行Get方式通信
下载apache包 http://hc.apache.org/downloads.cgi 比较eclipse自带api,简单,易上手 实例: package zw1; import java.io.I ...
- Html 经典布局(一)
经典布局案例(一): <!DOCTYPE html> <html lang="en"> <head> <meta charset=&quo ...
- struts2 之 ServletAPI
1. 在struts2中有两种方式使用SercletAPI,一种解耦方式,一种耦合方式. 2. 解耦方式就是使用ActionContext 来实现,是完全解耦 servletAPI. ActionCo ...
- QT链接数据库
在介绍QT与数据的链接问题上,我在这里就不介绍关于QT环境与mysql.sqlite3环境的安装步骤了,以下的所有的操作都是建立在你已经安装了所有环境的基础上的.好的,那我们就具体来看一看QT环境中怎 ...
- 交作业啊,python爬取58的页面
第一次写博文,好紧张啊,写这么烂怎么给别人看啊先做下总结: 刚开始学习python,自我感觉python写起来确实很方便,各种库,各种语法糖,不过刚接触,一下子记不下来这么多东西,总感觉乱乱的,用的多 ...
- nginx源码分析——http模块
源码:nginx 1.12.0 一.nginx http模块简介 由于nginx的性能优势,现在已经有越来越多的单位.个人采用nginx或者openresty. ...