https://www.ted.com/talks/emily_balcetis_why_some_people_find_exercise_harder_than_others/transcript

00:12
Vision is the most important and prioritized[praɪˈɔ:rətaɪz]优先处理 sense that we have. We are constantly looking at the world around us, and quickly we identify and make sense of what it is that we see.

00:25
Let's just start with an example of that very fact. I'm going to show you a photograph of a person, just for a second or two, and I'd like for you to identify what emotion is on his face. Ready? Here you go. Go with your gut[gʌt]本能的 reaction. Okay. What did you see? Well, we actually surveyed over 120 individuals, and the results were mixed. People did not agree on what emotion they saw on his face. Maybe you saw discomfort. That was the most frequent response that we received. But if you asked the person on your left, they might have said regret or skepticism[ˈskɛptɪˌsɪzəm]怀疑态度, and if you asked somebody on your right, they might have said something entirely different, like hope or empathy[ˈɛmpəθi]. So we are all looking at the very same face again. We might see something entirely different, because perception[pərˈsepʃn]知觉 is subjective. What we think we see is actually filtered['fɪltəd] through our own mind's eye.

01:29
Of course, there are many other examples of how we see the world through own mind's eye. I'm going to give you just a few. So dieters, for instance, see apples as larger than people who are not counting calories. Softball players see the ball as smaller if they've just come out of a slump[slʌmp]大幅度下降, compared to people who had a hot night at the plate. And actually, our political beliefs also can affect the way we see other people, including politicians[pɒlɪ'tɪʃnz]. So my research team and I decided to test this question. In 2008, Barack Obama was running for president for the very first time, and we surveyed hundreds of Americans one month before the election. What we found in this survey was that some people, some Americans, think photographs like these best reflect how Obama really looks. Of these people, 75 percent voted for Obama in the actual election. Other people, though, thought photographs like these best reflect how Obama really looks. 89 percent of these people voted for McCain. We presented many photographs of Obama one at a time, so people did not realize that what we were changing from one photograph to the next was whether we had artificially lightened or darkened his skin tone.

02:52
So how is that possible? How could it be that when I look at a person, an object, or an event, I see something very different than somebody else does? Well, the reasons are many, but one reason requires that we understand a little bit more about how our eyes work. So vision scientists know that the amount of information that we can see at any given point in time, what we can focus on, is actually relatively small. What we can see with great sharpness and clarity and accuracy is the equivalent of the surface area of our thumb on our outstretched arm. Everything else around that is blurry[ˈblə:ɪ]模糊的, rendering[ˈrɛndərɪŋ] much of what is presented to our eyes as ambiguous. But we have to clarify and make sense of what it is that we see, and it's our mind that helps us fill in that gap. As a result, perception is a subjective experience, and that's how we end up seeing through our own mind's eye.

03:53
So, I'm a social psychologist, and it's questions like these that really intrigue me. I am fascinated by those times when people do not see eye to eye. Why is it that somebody might literally see the glass as half full, and somebody literally sees it as half empty? What is it about what one person is thinking and feeling that leads them to see the world in an entirely different way? And does that even matter? So to begin to tackle these questions, my research team and I decided to delve[dɛlv]挖掘 deeply into an issue that has received international attention: our health and fitness. Across the world, people are struggling to manage their weight, and there is a variety of strategies that we have to help us keep the pounds off. For instance, we set the best of intentions to exercise after the holidays, but actually, the majority of Americans find that their New Year's resolutions are broken by Valentine's Day. We talk to ourselves in very encouraging ways, telling ourselves this is our year to get back into shape, but that is not enough to bring us back to our ideal weight. So why? Of course, there is no simple answer, but one reason, I argue, is that our mind's eye might work against us. Some people may literally[ˈlɪtərəli]真正地 see exercise as more difficult, and some people might literally see exercise as easier.

05:22
So, as a first step to testing these questions, we gathered objective measurements of individuals' physical fitness. We measured the circumference of their waist, compared to the circumference of their hips. A higher waist-to-hip ratio is an indicator of being less physically fit than a lower waist-to-hip ratio. After gathering these measurements, we told our participants that they would walk to a finish line while carrying extra weight in a sort of race. But before they did that, we asked them to estimate the distance to the finish line. We thought that the physical states of their body might change how they perceived the distance. So what did we find? Well, waist-to-hip ratio predicted perceptions of distance. People who were out of shape and unfit actually saw the distance to the finish line as significantly greater than people who were in better shape. People's states of their own body changed how they perceived the environment. But so too can our mind. In fact, our bodies and our minds work in tandem[ˈtændəm]串联 to change how we see the world around us.

06:31
That led us to think that maybe people with strong motivations and strong goals to exercise might actually see the finish line as closer than people who have weaker motivations. So to test whether motivations affect our perceptual experiences in this way, we conducted a second study. Again, we gathered objective measurements of people's physical fitness, measuring the circumference of their waist and the circumference of their hips, and we had them do a few other tests of fitness. Based on feedback that we gave them, some of our participants told us they're not motivated to exercise any more. They felt like they already met their fitness goals and they weren't going to do anything else. These people were not motivated. Other people, though, based on our feedback, told us they were highly motivated to exercise. They had a strong goal to make it to the finish line. But again, before we had them walk to the finish line, we had them estimate the distance. How far away was the finish line? And again, like the previous study, we found that waist-to-hip ratio predicted perceptions of distance. Unfit individuals saw the distance as farther, saw the finish line as farther away, than people who were in better shape. Importantly, though, this only happened for people who were not motivated to exercise. On the other hand, people who were highly motivated to exercise saw the distance as short. Even the most out of shape individuals saw the finish line as just as close, if not slightly closer, than people who were in better shape.

08:06
So our bodies can change how far away that finish line looks, but people who had committed to a manageable goal that they could accomplish in the near future and who believed that they were capable of meeting that goal actually saw the exercise as easier. That led us to wonder, is there a strategy that we could use and teach people that would help change their perceptions of the distance, help them make exercise look easier?

08:35
So we turned to the vision science literature to figure out what should we do, and based on what we read, we came up with a strategy that we called, "Keep your eyes on the prize." So this is not the slogan from an inspirational poster. It's an actual directive for how to look around your environment. People that we trained in this strategy, we told them to focus their attention on the finish line, to avoid looking around, to imagine a spotlight was shining on that goal, and that everything around it was blurry and perhaps difficult to see. We thought that this strategy would help make the exercise look easier. We compared this group to a baseline group. To this group we said, just look around the environment as you naturally would. You will notice the finish line, but you might also notice the garbage can off to the right, or the people and the lamp post off to the left. We thought that people who used this strategy would see the distance as farther.

09:34
So what did we find? When we had them estimate the distance, was this strategy successful for changing their perceptual experience? Yes. People who kept their eyes on the prize saw the finish line as 30 percent closer than people who looked around as they naturally would. We thought this was great. We were really excited because it meant that this strategy helped make the exercise look easier, but the big question was, could this help make exercise actually better? Could it improve the quality of exercise as well?

10:07
So next, we told our participants, you are going to walk to the finish line while wearing extra weight. We added weights to their ankles that amounted to 15 percent of their body weight. We told them to lift their knees up high and walk to the finish line quickly. We designed this exercise in particular to be moderately challenging but not impossible, like most exercises that actually improve our fitness.

10:32
So the big question, then: Did keeping your eyes on the prize and narrowly focusing on the finish line change their experience of the exercise? It did. People who kept their eyes on the prize told us afterward that it required 17 percent less exertion for them to do this exercise than people who looked around naturally. It changed their subjective experience of the exercise. It also changed the objective nature of their exercise. People who kept their eyes on the prize actually moved 23 percent faster than people who looked around naturally. To put that in perspective, a 23 percent increase is like trading in your 1980 Chevy Citation for a 1980 Chevrolet Corvette.

11:23
We were so excited by this, because this meant that a strategy that costs nothing, that is easy for people to use, regardless of whether they're in shape or struggling to get there, had a big effect. Keeping your eyes on the prize made the exercise look and feel easier even when people were working harder because they were moving faster. Now, I know there's more to good health than walking a little bit faster, but keeping your eyes on the prize might be one additional strategy that you can use to help promote a healthy lifestyle.

11:59
If you're not convinced yet that we all see the world through our own mind's eye, let me leave you with one final example. Here's a photograph of a beautiful street in Stockholm, with two cars. The car in the back looks much larger than the car in the front. However, in reality, these cars are the same size, but that's not how we see it. So does this mean that our eyes have gone haywire and that our brains are a mess? No, it doesn't mean that at all. It's just how our eyes work. We might see the world in a different way, and sometimes that might not line up with reality, but it doesn't mean that one of us is right and one of us is wrong. We all see the world through our mind's eye, but we can teach ourselves to see it differently.

12:46
So I can think of days that have gone horribly wrong for me. I'm fed up, I'm grumpy[ˈgrʌmpi]脾气暴躁的, I'm tired, and I'm so behind, and there's a big black cloud hanging over my head, and on days like these, it looks like everyone around me is down in the dumps too. My colleague at work looks annoyed when I ask for an extension on a deadline, and my friend looks frustrated when I show up late for lunch because a meeting ran long, and at the end of the day, my husband looks disappointed because I'd rather go to bed than go to the movies. And on days like these, when everybody looks upset and angry to me, I try to remind myself that there are other ways of seeing them. Perhaps my colleague was confused, perhaps my friend was concerned, and perhaps my husband was feeling empathy instead. So we all see the world through our own mind's eye, and on some days, it might look like the world is a dangerous and challenging and insurmountable[ˌɪnsərˈmaʊntəbl]不可逾越的 place, but it doesn't have to look that way all the time. We can teach ourselves to see it differently, and when we find a way to make the world look nicer and easier, it might actually become so.

13:57
Thank you.

13:59
(Applause)

(14)Why some people find exercise harder than others的更多相关文章

  1. 【Bell-Ford 算法】CLRS Exercise 24.1-4,24.1-6

    本文是一篇笔记,大部分内容取自 CLRS 第三版,第 24.1 节. Exercise 24.1-4 Modify the Bellman-Ford algorithm so that it sets ...

  2. C - The C Answer (2nd Edition) - Exercise 1-4

    /* Write a program to print the corresponding Celsius to Fahrenheit table. */ #include <stdio.h&g ...

  3. MIT 6.828 JOS学习笔记5. Exercise 1.3

    Lab 1 Exercise 3 设置一个断点在地址0x7c00处,这是boot sector被加载的位置.然后让程序继续运行直到这个断点.跟踪/boot/boot.S文件的每一条指令,同时使用boo ...

  4. 《how to design programs》14章 再论自引用数据

    这是一个家族谱: ;child(define-struct child (father mother name date eyes)) #lang racket ;child (define-stru ...

  5. CMSC 216 Exercise #5

    CMSC 216 Exercise #5 Spring 2019Shell Jr (”Shellito”) Due: Tue Apr 23, 2019, 11:30PM1 ObjectivesTo p ...

  6. 软件测试:3.Exercise Section 2.3

    软件测试:3.Exercise Section 2.3 /************************************************************ * Finds an ...

  7. 《学习OpenCV3》第14章课后习题

    1.在一条含有 N 个点的封闭轮廓中,我们可以通过比较每个点与其它点的距离,找出最外层的点.(这个翻译有问题,而且这个问题是实际问题) a.这样一个算法的复杂度是多少? b.怎样用更快的速度完成这个任 ...

  8. 18 A GIF decoder: an exercise in Go interfaces 一个GIF解码器:go语言接口训练

    A GIF decoder: an exercise in Go interfaces  一个GIF解码器:go语言接口训练 25 May 2011 Introduction At the Googl ...

  9. 《MIT 6.828 Lab 1 Exercise 8》实验报告

    本实验的网站链接:MIT 6.828 Lab 1 Exercise 8. 题目 Exercise 8. Read through kern/printf.c, lib/printfmt.c, and ...

随机推荐

  1. TZOJ 2722 Matrix(树状数组区间取反单点查询)

    描述 Given an N*N matrix A, whose elements are either 0 or 1. A[i, j] means the number in the i-th row ...

  2. TZOJ 数据结构期末历年题目

    A.数据结构练习题――线性表操作 线性表的基本操作 1.在某个位置p插入val,复杂度O(p) 2.在某个位置p删除val,复杂度O(p) 3.查找某个位置p的值,复杂度O(p) 4.清除链表,复杂度 ...

  3. f5版本升级

    1)上传系统IOS及Hotfix 点击import按钮,选择要上传的文件.上传成功的话就会显示上传成功的10.2.4的iso文件 2)通过CLI命令行方式上传补丁 通过SSH工具将ISO以及Hotfi ...

  4. classLoader.getResourceAsStream中文乱码

    一直用一个方法安然无恙,今天在新项目中突然乱码了,原代码: ClassLoader classLoader = Thread.currentThread().getContextClassLoader ...

  5. Day 07 文件的相关操作

    文件初始: 文件的三要素: path:文件的路径 mode:r w r+ w+ a encoding: 编码方式 # 打开一个文件的方法 f1 = open('e:\echo.txt', encodi ...

  6. shell条件控制和循环结构

    一.简介 Shell编程中循环命令用于特定条件下决定某些语句重复执行的控制方式,有三种常用的循环语句:for.while和until.while循环和for循环属于“当型循环”,而until属于“直到 ...

  7. JSP中的数据库操作,MySQL基础操作(一)

    一.JDBC JDBC(java data base concectivity),是一种用于执行SQL语句的java API,可以为多种关系库提供统一访问. 通常使用JDBC完成以下操作: 1)同数据 ...

  8. 修复python命令行下接收不到参数的问题

    由于之前安装过多个python版本,导致出现了在命令行下直接给py文件传递参数的时候接收不到,即使重新卸载安装也没有解决. 解决办法: 修改如下图路径下的键值为:"D:\Python27\p ...

  9. Vmware黑屏解决方法

    此原因是显卡性能差,显示选项开启了3D加速导致的,具体修改步骤: 英文路径:VM->Settings->Hardware->Display 在右面的内容栏中将 Accelerate ...

  10. mybatis进阶--输入映射和输出映射

    我们知道,mapper.xml是我们配置操作数据库的sql语句的地方.其中每个sql语句对应着一个方法,每个方法都有自己的输入输出参数类型.那么这些类型都是怎么配置的呢?今天我们来一起学习下. 输入映 ...